Age, Biography and Wiki

Ap Dijksterhuis was born on 12 November, 1968 in Zutphen, Netherlands, is an A dutch social psychologists. Discover Ap Dijksterhuis's Biography, Age, Height, Physical Stats, Dating/Affairs, Family and career updates. Learn How rich is he in this year and how he spends money? Also learn how he earned most of networth at the age of 55 years old?

Popular As N/A
Occupation N/A
Age 55 years old
Zodiac Sign Scorpio
Born 12 November, 1968
Birthday 12 November
Birthplace Zutphen, Netherlands
Nationality Netherlands

We recommend you to check the complete list of Famous People born on 12 November. He is a member of famous with the age 55 years old group.

Ap Dijksterhuis Height, Weight & Measurements

At 55 years old, Ap Dijksterhuis height not available right now. We will update Ap Dijksterhuis's Height, weight, Body Measurements, Eye Color, Hair Color, Shoe & Dress size soon as possible.

Physical Status
Height Not Available
Weight Not Available
Body Measurements Not Available
Eye Color Not Available
Hair Color Not Available

Dating & Relationship status

He is currently single. He is not dating anyone. We don't have much information about He's past relationship and any previous engaged. According to our Database, He has no children.

Family
Parents Not Available
Wife Not Available
Sibling Not Available
Children Not Available

Ap Dijksterhuis Net Worth

His net worth has been growing significantly in 2023-2024. So, how much is Ap Dijksterhuis worth at the age of 55 years old? Ap Dijksterhuis’s income source is mostly from being a successful . He is from Netherlands. We have estimated Ap Dijksterhuis's net worth, money, salary, income, and assets.

Net Worth in 2024 $1 Million - $5 Million
Salary in 2024 Under Review
Net Worth in 2023 Pending
Salary in 2023 Under Review
House Not Available
Cars Not Available
Source of Income

Ap Dijksterhuis Social Network

Instagram
Linkedin
Twitter Ap Dijksterhuis Twitter
Facebook
Wikipedia Ap Dijksterhuis Wikipedia
Imdb

Timeline

1968

Albert Jan "Ap" Dijksterhuis (born 12 November 1968, Zutphen) is a Dutch Social Psychologist at Radboud University Nijmegen.

1996

He received his Ph.D in Social Sciences from Radboud University Nijmegen in 1996.

His adviser was Ad van Knippenberg.

From 1996-1999, he did post-doc work as a Research Fellow of the Royal Dutch Academy of Arts and Sciences, located in Amsterdam.

2000

In 2000, he became a professor at the University of Amsterdam, returning to Radboud University Nijmegen in 2006.

2004

Unconscious thought theory (UTT) was introduced by Dijksterhuis in 2004.

He presented 5 experiments showing that people make better decisions when they thought about it unconsciously rather than consciously.

The argument is that conscious thought is not capable of handling all the complex information that we need to process in order to make good decisions.

In experiment 2 participants in the conscious thought condition reported only using a subset of the information provided to make their decision.

2006

In a 2006 paper, Dijksterhuis and colleagues did multiple studies looking at making the best choices.

In the first study, participants were shown 4 (simple) or 12 (complex) attributes, randomized one at a time, about four fake cars.

The attributes were either positive or negative.

Two of the cars had equal numbers of positive and negative attributes, one was presented with 75% positive attributes, and the other 25%.

2007

In 2007, his first book was published, in Dutch, called ‘Het slimme onbewuste’ (‘The Smart Unconscious’).

His areas of research have varied, but all deal with unconscious: the perception-behavior link, goals, Implicit Self Esteem Issues, and unconscious thought.

He works & co-leads the Unconscious lab at Radboud University Nijmegen, with Rick van Baaren.

The lab’s primary focus is on unconscious thought and imitation.

His theory that certain behaviour can be modified by unconscious cues is somewhat controversial.

Bargh, Chen, and Burrows showed a relationship between activation of traits and behavior.

In one of the experiments they primed participants with words related to the elderly and found they walked slower leaving the experiment than controls.

Dijksterhuis started investigating the link between perception and behavior, but instead of direct behavior he wanted to measure ability.

Over multiple studies, Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg primed subjects with the stereotype of professor (thought of as intelligent), and the stereotype of soccer hooligans (thought of as stupid).

In the last study, they also primed them directly with intelligent or stupid, rather than just using a stereotype.

In a supposedly unrelated task, they used a general knowledge measure to test for effects of the priming.

In all of the studies, participants primed with intelligence, by stereotype or directly, did better than controls.

The opposite is true for those primed with stupidity.

The duration of priming (9 minutes vs 2 minutes) affected performance with those in the long condition performing better than those in the short condition.

These results have been called into question due to failure of multiple researchers to replicate.

Much work has been done on measuring self-esteem.

More recently, there has been a focus on implicit self-esteem, and how to measure it.

According to Bosson, Swann, and Pennebaker there is little to no correlation between several previously used implicit self-esteem measures.

The same results were found when comparing implicit and explicit measures.

Dijksterhuis acknowledges this limitation and tries to work around it by manipulating implicit self-esteem then measuring it with the name-letter effect or self-esteem IAT.

In the experiments, he uses evaluative conditioning to enhance implicit self-esteem subconsciously.

The word ‘I’ was subliminally presented (15 milliseconds) followed by either positive traits (e.g. smart) or neutral words (e.g. chair), depending on the condition.

Evaluative conditioning is most often used to change attitude towards a neutral stimuli, but he proposed it would work for implicit self-esteem.

The results support his hypothesis; participants in the experimental condition showed higher levels of implicit self-esteem on both measures compared to controls.

Greenwald and Banaji’s definition of implicit self-esteem is: “implicit self-esteem is the introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) effect of the self-attitude on evaluation of self-associated and self-dissociated objects”.

Dijksterhuis proposes an alternate definition that self-esteem is the attitude and that the evaluation of objects is a consequence of this attitude.

He also proposes that the different measures have not correlated with each other because they are either measuring the attitude towards self or the consequence of this attitude.