Age, Biography and Wiki

Esther Thelen was born on 20 May, 1941 in Brooklyn, New York, United States, is an An american cognitive scientists. Discover Esther Thelen's Biography, Age, Height, Physical Stats, Dating/Affairs, Family and career updates. Learn How rich is she in this year and how she spends money? Also learn how she earned most of networth at the age of 63 years old?

Popular As N/A
Occupation N/A
Age 63 years old
Zodiac Sign Taurus
Born 20 May 1941
Birthday 20 May
Birthplace Brooklyn, New York, United States
Date of death 29 December, 2004
Died Place Bloomington, Indiana
Nationality United States

We recommend you to check the complete list of Famous People born on 20 May. She is a member of famous with the age 63 years old group.

Esther Thelen Height, Weight & Measurements

At 63 years old, Esther Thelen height not available right now. We will update Esther Thelen's Height, weight, Body Measurements, Eye Color, Hair Color, Shoe & Dress size soon as possible.

Physical Status
Height Not Available
Weight Not Available
Body Measurements Not Available
Eye Color Not Available
Hair Color Not Available

Dating & Relationship status

She is currently single. She is not dating anyone. We don't have much information about She's past relationship and any previous engaged. According to our Database, She has no children.

Family
Parents Not Available
Husband Not Available
Sibling Not Available
Children Not Available

Esther Thelen Net Worth

Her net worth has been growing significantly in 2023-2024. So, how much is Esther Thelen worth at the age of 63 years old? Esther Thelen’s income source is mostly from being a successful . She is from United States. We have estimated Esther Thelen's net worth, money, salary, income, and assets.

Net Worth in 2024 $1 Million - $5 Million
Salary in 2024 Under Review
Net Worth in 2023 Pending
Salary in 2023 Under Review
House Not Available
Cars Not Available
Source of Income

Esther Thelen Social Network

Instagram
Linkedin
Twitter
Facebook
Wikipedia
Imdb

Timeline

1941

Esther Thelen (May 20, 1941 – December 29, 2004) was an expert in the field of developmental psychology.

Thelen's research was focused on human development, especially in the area of infant development.

Thelen was also president of the Society for Research in Child Development and the International Society for Infant Studies.

She was a Fellow of the American Association for the Advancement of Science and American Psychological Society.

Thelen is known for her works on infant development, particularly those that focused on complex movement and behavioral development.

Thelen applied chaos theory to the research of how babies learn to walk and interact with the world around them.

In Thelen's view behavior emerges as a pattern from all the streams that flow into the river of infant development.

Or, as she wrote "The mind simply does not exist as something decoupled from the body and experience".

She suggested that an infant already has basic motor patterns at birth as demonstrated by stepping reflex and spontaneous kicking.

Thelen's works expanded Gerald Edelman's model of development by proposing a broader conceptualization of development.

Edelman, in his theory of neuronal development, showed that development occurs in the brain between neuro-networks that overlap and interconnect.

The epigenetic process of neural development is grounded in the idea of experience-dependent changes which is development or growth by selectively and simultaneously reinforcing neural pathways.

As children, humans are constantly moving and interacting.

Visual and kinematic information becomes mapped together in the brain and the pathways are strengthened and retained through every interaction.

When the child encounters a novel or new skill, the child takes a similar previously learned motor map and applies it to the new novel skill.

As the novel skill develops into a new behavior, it then in turn can be used to help develop future skills.

Recurrent activities in the world reinforce this Dynamical systems theory of development and helps explain the constructivist view of the Developmental Systems Theory.

Thelen's contribution in this area involves the notion that the nature of physical development is not absolute but flexible.

According to Thelen, development is self-reorganization (with the self pertaining to the system instead of the psychoanalytic self) that emerges due to the interaction of the system/organization/person with another or the environment.

Edelman used the terms reentrant and degenerate in order to describe these complex neural connections.

Reentrant was defined as a complex interwoven system in which the output can feed back into the input.

Reentry is a two way straight that can run in parallel.

Degenerate was explained by saying that the pathway can be jointly determined by multiple causes and isn't determined by just one thing.

This joint determination by multiple causality is one major theme of developmental systems theory that also overlaps with the dynamical systems theory by Esther Thelen.

An example of how multiple causes can lead to one action is human movement.

In the body, the brain can send many different signals to cause movements such as speech.

These signals sent from the brain go to many different muscles in order to control the movement as we speak a word.

Figure 3 (shown at right) shows in a very simplistic way the directionality of the neural connections.

Esther Thelen used A-not-B error as an example of how movement is complex.

In figure 6, Esther Thelen illustrated the use of movement parameters in the decision for the infant to move to either A position or B position.

As stated in the dynamical systems theory in order for movement to occur, the control parameter must be scaled up above the threshold.

The input given to the child by the placing of the toy or the hinting at a certain cover is the specific input.

This also factors into the decisions needing to be made in order for the child to make a movement towards the toy.

Making a decision as to where to move and the actual movement towards the toy is time dependent.

Three aspects factor into the time taken in making this decision and making the movement.

These factors include transiet and tonic visual input and the infants memory of the toy location.

The task input begins with the first movement towards the toy.

After the second time for the child to reach for the toy after it has been hidden under the cover the child is using specific input.

This specific input is from their memory of their first time reaching for the toy.

This input is displayed in Figure 7.